Discussion

Law Of Attraction For Kids

Smart Parenting Guide

Get Instant Access

In a broad sense, parents who habitually engage in more nonpresent and related-to-present talk with their young children provide more opportunities for children to learn to represent past events, to report intentions, feelings, and reactions, and to tie experiences in one context to those in another. All of these, we believe, are skills that are critical to later autonomous narrative production. As children become more capable of extended nonpresent talk, parents' topic extensions begin to include more specific prompts for information about narrative participants, setting, consequences, and resolutions, elements of narrative structure that our most successful children were able to produce independently at age 5. Thus, the relationships demonstrated between nonpresent talk at 20 and 32 months and the production of narrative structure and evaluation at 5 years of age, both in personal narrative and in fantasy play, reflect interactional histories that go beyond specific narrative exchanges. As Peterson and McCabe (1992, 1994) and Fivush and Hayden have argued (Fivush, 1994; Hayden & Fivush 1997), young children acquire understandings of culturally important elements of narrative structure through participation in joint conversation about past events with parents. The present study suggests that this interactional picture should be broadened to include other kinds of talk about the nonpresent (talk about nonobservable feelings and thoughts, for example), as well as talk about the related-to-present.

Pragmatic flexibility assesses another feature of extended parent-child talk, the ability to engage in a variety of communicative activities and to shift between here-and-now and nonpresent frames of reference. This aspect of interactional experience is only moderately associated with children's later ability to produce personal narrative, but shows stronger associations with the ability to participate successfully in fantasy play. Unlike personal narrative, fantasy play is a context that requires frequent shifts between talk with the adult partner in the immediate context of joint pretense (Oops, the lion's tail fell off!), relatively autonomous narration of story happenings (Out jumped the lion cub's mommy), and shifts into the voice of story characters. (And the dragon said to all the other animals, "I don't like you. I'm gonna eat you all up.") The ability to manage these shifts in narrative role may be facilitated by early practice in shifting frames of reference in conversation with parents.

Early morphosyntactic attainment as indexed by mean length of utterance and by IPSyn at 32 months shows little association with any of the areas of extended discourse competence at 5 years of age, though there is a hint of involvement in children's construction of a fantasy world. This may preview a larger role for morphosyntax as children begin to use more sophisticated anaphoric strategies for marking shifts in focus between central and peripheral story characters, and as they more consistently use anchor tense to bind together events in the story world.

The picture that emerges, then, as we consider continuity between early and later pragmatic skills is not simply one of homotypic continuity, with early experience in relating past events or engaging in fantasy narration with an adult partner predicting later personal narrative and fantasy narrative skills. Rather, it is also one of heterotypic continuity, in which experience in talking about the nonpresent, practice in negotiating shifts between talk about the here-and-now and talk about the nonpresent, and engagement in a broad range of communicative interchanges, may be crucial preparation for later autonomous narrative production.

FUTURE DIRECTIONS

The work described here is limited in a number of respects and only begins to scratch the surface of children's abilities to tell different kinds of stories. As is true of the vast bulk of spontaneous-language based research, the dyads studied here were primarily from white, English-speaking, middle-class families. Data on parent-child conversations in low-income and racial/ethnic minority families are woefully scarce, and for the most part have not been analyzed from a pragmatic perspective; we know little about the early language experiences of children in such families and how those experiences may be related to narrative and other discourse skills children bring to kindergarten. Greater depth of information is also needed about the naturally-occurring fantasy talk of children from all backgrounds in interaction with peers and siblings, and in small group as well as dyadic contexts. Mother-child verbal interaction, though indisputably important, is only one of many communicative participant structures most toddlers experience. Talk with fathers and other caregivers, siblings, and familiar peers undoubtedly each provides grist for the mill as toddlers and preschoolers come to understand and verbalize the physical (sometimes not present) world, multiple social worlds, and universe of fantasy worlds. Narrative offers a window on children's emerging understandings of such worlds and on the linguistic skills with which they navigate among them.

REFERENCES

Aukrust, V. G., & Snow, C. E. (1998). Narratives and explanations in Norwegian and American mealtime conversations. Language in Society, 27(2), 221-246.

Bartlett, F. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Berman, R. A., & Slobin, D. I. (1994). Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Bishop, D. M., & Edmundson, A. (1987). Language impaired four year olds: Distinguishing transient from persistent impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52, 156-173.

Blum-Kulka, S. (1997). Dinner talk: Cultural patterns of sociability and socialization in family discourse. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Blum-Kulka, S., & Snow, C. (1992). Developing autonomy for tellers, tales and telling in family narrative-events. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 2, 187-217.

Botvin, G., & Sutton-Smith, B. (1977). The development of structural complexity in children's fantasy narratives. Developmental Psychology, 13(4), 377-388.

Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Cazden, C. (1988). Classroom discourse: The language of teaching and learning. Portsmouth, NH: Heineman.

Dale, P., Bates, E., Reznick, J., & Morisset, C. (1989). The validity of a parent report instrument of child language at twenty months. Journal of Child Language, 16, 239-249.

Eisenberg, A. (1985). Learning to describe past experiences in conversation. Discourse Processes, 8, 177-204.

Feagans, L., & Applebaum, M. I. (1986). Validation of language subtypes in learning disabled children. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 78, 358-364.

Fivush, R. (1991). The social construction of personal narratives. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 37, 59-82.

Fivush, R., & Hudson, J. (1990). Knowing and remembering in young children. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Gergen, K. J. (1994). Realities and Relationships: Soundings in Social Construction. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Universtiy Press.

Gumperz, J. J. & Hymes, D. (1972). Directions in sociolinguis-tics: The ethnography of communication. New York: Holt, Reinhart, & Winston.

Haight, W., & Miller, P. (1993). Pretending at home. Albany: SUNY Press.

Han, J., Leichtman, M., & Wang, Q. (1998). Autobiographical memory in Korean, Chinese, and American children. Developmental Psychology, 34, 701-713.

Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways with words: Language, life and work in communities and classrooms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Hemphill, L., Feldman, H., Camp, L., & Griffin, T. (1994). Developmental changes in narrative and non-narrative discourse in children with and without brain injury. Journal of Communication Disorders, 27, 107-133.

Hemphill, L., & Snow, C. (1996). Language and literacy development: Discontinuities and differences. In D. Olson & N. Torrance (Eds.), The handbook of education and human development: New models of learning, teaching and schooling (pp. 173-201). Oxford:Blackwell Publishers.

Hollingshead, A. (1975). The four-factor index of social status. Unpublished manuscript. Yale University, Department of Sociology, New Haven, CT.

Hudson, J. (1990). The emergence of autobiographical memory in mother-child conversation. In R. Fivush & J. Hudson (Eds.), Knowing and remembering in young children (pp. 166-196). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Hudson, J. (1993). Reminiscing with mothers and others: Autobiographical memory in young two-year-olds. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 3, 1-32.

Hudson, J., & Shapiro, L. (1991). From knowing to telling: The development of children's scripts, stories, and personal narratives. In A. McCabe & C. Peterson (Eds.), Developing narrative structure (pp. 89-136). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Hughes, C., & Dunn, J. (1998). Understanding mind and emotion: Longitudinal associations with mental-state talk between young friends. Developmental Psychology, 34(5), 1026-1037.

Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1967). Narrative analysis: Oral versions of personal experience. In J. Helm (Ed.), Essays on the verbal and visual arts (pp. 12-44). Seattle: University of Washington Press.

MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for analyzing talk. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

McCabe, A. (1996). Chameleon readers: Teaching children how to appreciate all kinds of good stories. New York: McGraw-Hill.

McCabe, A., & Peterson, C. (1991). Developing narrative structure. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Mead, G. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

Melzi, G. (2000). Cultural variations in the construction of personal narratives: Central American and European American mothers' elicitation styles. Discourse Processes, 30, 153-177.

Michaels, S. (1981). Sharing time: Children's narrative styles and differential access to literacy. Language and Society, 10, 423-442.

Miller, P. J. (1982). Amy, Wendy, and Beth: Learning Lan guage in South Baltimore. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.

Miller, P. J., Potts, R., Fung, H., Hoogstra, L., & Mintz, J. (1990). Narrative practices and the social construction of self in childhood. American Ethnologist, 17(2), 292-311.

Miller, P. J., Mintz, J., Hoogstra, L., Fung, H., & Potts, R., (1992). The narrated self: Young children's construction of self in relations to others in conversational stories of personal experience. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 38(1), 45-67.

Miller, P., Sandel, T., Liang, C., & Fung, H. (2001). Narrating transgressions in Longwood: The discourses, meanings, and paradoxes of an American socializing practice. Ethos, 29(2), 159-186.

Minami, M. (2001). Maternal styles of narrative elicitation and the development of children's narrative skill: A study on parental scaffolding. Narrative Inquiry, 11, 55-80.

Minami, M., & McCabe, A. (1991). Haiku as a discourse regulation device: A stanza analysis of Japanese children's personal narratives. Language in Society, 20, 577-599.

Mullen, M. (1994). Earliest recollections of childhood: A demographic analysis. Cognition, 52, 55-79.

Mullen, M., & Yi, S. (1995). The cultural context of talk about the past: Implications for the development of autobiographical memory. Cognitive Development, 40, 407-419.

Nelson, K. (1986). Event knowledge: Structure and function in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Nelson, K. (1989). Narratives from the crib. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development: Emergence of the mediated mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Nelson, K. (2000). Narrative, time and the emergence of the encultured self. Culture & Psychology, 6(2), 183-196.

Nelson, K. (2001). Language and the self: From the "Experiencing I" to the "Continuing Me". In C. Moore, & K. Lemmon (Eds.), The self in time: Developmental perspectives (pp. 15-33). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2000). Socialization of memory. In E. Tulving & F. Craik (Eds.), Handbook of memory (pp. 283-295). New York: Oxford University Press.

Ninio, A. (1993). On the fringes of the system: Children's acquisition of syntactically isolated forms at the onset of speech. First Language, 13, 291-314.

Ninio, A., & Snow, C. E. (1996). Pragmatic Development. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

Ninio, A., Snow, C. E., Pan, B. A., & Rollins, P. (1994). Classifying communicative acts in children's interactions. Journal of Communications Disorders, 27, 157-188.

Ninio, A., & Wheeler, P. (1984). A manual for classifying verbal communicative acts in mother-infant interaction. Working Papers in Developmental Psychology, No. 1. Jerusalem: The Martin and Vivian Levin Center, Hebrew University. Reprinted as Transcript Analysis, 1986, 3, 1-82.

Ochs, E., & Capps, L. (2001). Living narrative: Creating lives in everyday storytelling. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. (1984). Language acquisition and socialization: Three developmental stories and their application. In R. Shweder & R. A. Levine (Eds.), Culture theory (pp. 276-323). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Ochs, E., Taylor, C., Rudolph, D., & Smith, R. (1992). Storytelling as a theory-building activity. Discourse Processes, 15, 37-72.

Oppenheim, D., Emde, R., & Wamboldt, F. (1996). Associations between 3-year-olds' narrative co-constructions with mothers and fathers and their story completions about affective themes. Early Development & Parenting, 5(3), 149-160.

Oppenheim, D., Nir, A., Warren, S., & Emde, R. N. (1997). Emotion regulation in mother-child narrative co-construction: Associations with children's narratives and adaptation. Developmental Psychology, 33, 284-294.

Pan, B. A., Imbens-Bailey, A., Winner, K., & Snow, C. E. (1996). Communicative intents expressed by parents in interaction with young children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42, 72-90.

Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycho-linguistics: Three ways of looking at a child's narrative. New York: Plenum.

Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1992). Parental styles of narrative elicitation: Effect on children's narrative structure and content. First Language, 12, 299-321.

Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1994). A social interactionist account of developing decontextualized narrative skill. Developmental Psychology, 30, 937-948.

Pillemer, D., & White, S. (1989). Childhood events recalled by children and adults. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 21, pp. 297-340).

Reese, E. (1995). Predicting children's literacy from mother-child conversations. Cognitive Development, 10, 381-405.

Reese, E., Haden, C., & Fivush, R. (1993). Mother-child conversations about the past: Relationships of style and memory over time. Cognitive Development, 8, 403-430.

Roth, F., Speece, D. L. , Cooper, D. H., & De La Paz, S. (1996). Unresolved mysteries: How do metalinguistic and narra tive skills connect with early reading? Journal of Special Education, 30, 257-277.

Sachs, J., Goldman, J., & Chaille, C. (1984). Planning in pretend play: Using language to coordinate narrative development. In A. D. Pellegrini & T. D. Yawkey (Eds.), Advances in discourse processes: Vol. 13. The development of oral and written language in social contexts (pp. 119-128). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Scarborough, H. (1990). Index of productive syntax. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 1-22.

Snow, C. E. (1983). Literacy and language: Relationships during the preschool years. Harvard Educational Review, 53, 165-189.

Snow, C. E., Pan, B. A., Imbens-Bailey, A., & Herman, J. (1996). Learning how to say what one means: A longitudinal study of children's speech act use. Social Development, 5, 56-84.

Speece, D., Roth, F., Cooper, D. H., & de la Paz, S. (1999). The relevance of oral language skills to early literacy: A multivariate analysis. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 167-190.

Stern, D. (1989). Crib monologues from a psychoanalytic perspective. In K. Nelson (Ed.), Narratives from the crib (pp. 309-319). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Tessler, M., & Nelson, K. (1994). Making memories: The influence of joint encoding on later recall. Consciousness and Cognition, 3, 307-326

Vygotsky, L. (1934/1986). Thought and Language (A. Kozulin, Trans.). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Wolf, D., & Polanyi, L. (1990, October). Walking into story: A five year old stumbles on the nature of semantic worlds. Paper presented at the Conference on Language Development, Boston.

Part III

Childhood

Was this article helpful?

0 0
Confident Kids

Confident Kids

Although nobody gets a parenting manual or bible in the delivery room, it is our duty as parents to try to make our kids as well rounded, happy and confident as possible. It is a lot easier to bring up great kids than it is to try and fix problems caused by bad parenting, when our kids have become adults. Our children are all individuals - they are not our property but people in their own right.

Get My Free Ebook


Post a comment